CC..png    

Legal and postal addresses of the publisher: office 1336, 17 Naberezhnaya Severnoy Dviny, Arkhangelsk, 163002, Russian Federation, Northern (Arctic) Federal University named after M.V. Lomonosov

Phone: (818-2) 21-61-21
E-mail: vestnik_med@narfu.ru
https://vestnikmed.ru/en/

ABOUT JOURNAL

Latency of Visually Guided Saccades in Children Aged 2–9 Years. P. 217–225

Версия для печати

Section: Physiology

Download (pdf, 0.5MB )

UDC

612.846:612.66

DOI

10.37482/2687-1491-Z012

Authors

Ekaterina A. Budenkova* ORCID: 0000-0003-4854-5459
Dar’ya A. Shvayko* ORCID: 0000-0001-7622-5853
*Immanuel Kant Baltic Federal University (Kaliningrad, Russian Federation)
Corresponding author: Ekaterina Budenkova, address: ul. Universitetskaya 2, Kaliningrad, 236040, Russian Federation; e-mail: KBudenkova@gmail.com

Abstract

A great number of studies on saccades in children deal with neuropathology, while much less is known about saccades in healthy children. This research aimed to explore the yearly latency and the gap-effect characteristics of horizontal and vertical (both centrifugal and centripetal) visually guided saccades in healthy children. The study involved 418 subjects aged between 2 and 9 years. This age range was chosen due to the fact that during this postnatal period the processes of neuronal differentiation are most intensive. The RED250mobile eye tracker was used to record saccadic eye movements. The target saccade amplitude was 4.5° in response to dot stimuli in the Step and Gap paradigms. A correlation was found between the childʼs age and the parameters of centripetal saccades, while no age effect was identified for centrifugal saccades between 2 and 9 years. The parameters of centrifugal saccades remain stable between the age of 2 and 9 years. The vertical and horizontal saccades showed no statistically significant differences in latency and gap-effect. This indicates a parallel development of structures mediating vertical and horizontal saccades between 2 and 9 years. Asymmetry was observed in the groups of 7–9-year-olds (in vertical centrifugal saccades) and in 6-year-olds (horizontal saccades). This asymmetry could be caused by the heterochronic development of different oculomotor areas in the brain. Our results can contribute to the study of age-related saccadic dynamics and help to create a saccadic control database.

Keywords

saccade latency, saccadic gap effect, oculomotor activity in children, eye tracking, videooculography, centrifugal saccades, centripetal saccades

References

1. Rommelse N.N., Van der Stigchel S., Sergeant J.A. A Review on Eye Movement Studies in Childhood and Adolescent Psychiatry. Brain Cogn., 2008, vol. 68, no. 3, pp. 391–414.
2. Leigh R.J., Zee D.S. The Neurology of Eye Movements. New York, 2015. 1109 p.
3. Yakovlev P.L., Lecours A.R. The Myelogenetic Cycles of Regional Maturation of the Brain. Minkowski A. (ed.). Regional Development of the Brain in Early Life. Oxford, 1967, pp. 3–70.
4. Lemoine-Lardennois C., Alahyane N., Tailhefer C., Collins T., Fagard J., Doré-Mazars K. Saccadic Adaptation in 10–41-Month-Old Children. Front. Hum. Neurosci., 2016, no. 10, pp. 1–13.
5. Irving E.L., Steinbach M.J., Lillakas L., Babu R.J., Hutchings N. Horizontal Saccade Dynamics Across the Human Life Span. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci., 2006, vol. 47, no. 6, pp. 2478−2484.
6. Kovarski K., Siwiaszczyk M., Malvy J., Batty M., Latinus M. Faster Eye Movements in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Autism Res., 2019, vol. 12, no. 2, pp. 212–224.
7. Fukushima J., Hatta T., Fukushima K. Development of Voluntary Control of Saccadic Eye Movements: I. Age-Related Changes in Normal Children. Brain Dev., 2000, vol. 22, no. 3, pp. 173–180.
8. Eenshuistra R.M., Ridderinkhof K.R., Weidema M.A., van der Molen M.W. Developmental Changes in Oculomotor Control and Working-Memory Efficiency. Acta Psychol. (Amst.), 2007, vol. 124, no. 1, pp. 139–158.
9. Salman M.S., Sharpe J.A., Eizenman M., Lillakas L., Westall C., To T., Dennis M., Steinbach M.J. Saccades in Children. Vision Res., 2006, vol. 46, no. 8-9, pp. 1432−1439.
10. Alloway T.P., Alloway R.G. (eds.). Working Memory: The Connected Intelligence. New York, 2013. 321 p.
11. Bucci M.P., Seassau M. Vertical Saccades in Children: A Developmental Study. Exp. Brain Res., 2014, vol. 232, no. 3, pp. 927–934.
12. Casey B.J., Tottenham N., Liston C., Durston S. Imaging the Developing Brain: What Have We Learned About Cognitive Development? Trends Cogn. Sci., 2005, vol. 9, no. 3, pp. 104–110.
13. Gibson K.R., Petersen A.C. (eds.). Brain Maturation and Cognitive Development: Comparative and Cross-Cultural Perspectives. Routledge, 2017. 390 p.
14. Irving E.L., Lillakas L. Difference Between Vertical and Horizontal Saccades Across the Human Lifespan. Exp. Eye Res., 2019, vol. 183, pp. 38–45.
15. Hopf S., Liesenfeld M., Schmidtmann I., Ashayer S., Pitz S. Age Dependent Normative Data of Vertical and Horizontal Reflexive Saccades. PLoS One, 2018, vol. 13, no. 9. Art. no. e0204008.
16. Gaertner C., Wiener-Vacher S., Kapoula Z. Up/Down Anisotropies of Vertical Saccades in Healthy Children Depending on the Mode and the Depth of Execution. Int. J. Dev. Neurosci., 2016, vol. 52, pp. 82−92.
17. Tiadi A., Seassau M., Bui-Quoc E., Gerard C.-L., Bucci M.P. Vertical Saccades in Dyslexic Children. Res. Dev. Disabil., 2014, vol. 35, no. 11, pp. 3175−3181.
18. Matiz A., Crescentini C., Fabbro A., Budai R., Bergamasco M., Fabbro F. Spontaneous Eye Movements During Focused-Attention Mindfulness Meditation. PLoS One, 2019, vol. 14, no. 1. Art. no. e0210862.
19. Becker W., Jürgens R. Human Oblique Saccades: Quantitative Analysis of the Relation Between Horizontal and Vertical Components. Vision Res., 1990, vol. 30, no. 6, pp. 893–920.



Make a Submission


INDEXED IN: 

DOAJ_logo-colour.png

Elibrary.ru

logotype.png

infobaseindex

Логотип.png




Лань

OTHER NArFU JOURNALS: 

Vestnik of NArFU.
Series "Humanitarian and Social Sciences"

Forest Journal 
Лесной журнал 

Arctic and North